Why would you or anyone wish to out perform high IQ individuals? Well, for starters, all of us would like to be cleverer than we are. It is perhaps to inflate our egos, or it is perhaps to boost the functional utility of our lives. Either way, this inherent desire in us is more common than we’d like to admit.
But to become more intelligent, one need not compare oneself with “high-IQ individuals”. So, why is it that I specifically target outperforming such individuals in this essay? You see, I use it as a proxy to get into a far more valuable meta-discussion on different types of intelligent agents.
What we will be really discussing in this essay is the notion of hidden intelligence that is accessible to everyone with or without a high intelligence quotient (IQ). And that is the secret to becoming truly clever.
The unfortunate (or fortunate) fact is that the concepts that I will be discussing in this essay are hidden in plain sight. Anyone would be able to see them if the really wanted to. In other words, these are seriously undervalued concepts. Without any further ado, let us get started with the discussion.
Talking about secrets, it is no secret that humanity has made huge leaps in scientific progress in the last couple of centuries. As a consequence, we have begun understanding the value of investing time and effort in the practice and advancement of science.
A direct consequence of this understanding is the development of our present educational system in the form of schools and universities. Students are typically taught the basic sciences in school. Following school, students move to colleges and universities. The elites go on to complete their master’s degrees and doctoral theses.
All these systems are presently working well in the context of the aforementioned practice and advancement of science. However, there is one big secret hidden in plain sight here. Almost all of these systems such as schools and universities focus on top-down knowledge.
For any of us who was born in the past century, it might appear as if this is how science works and has always worked. But history tells us otherwise (history is also ironically a subject taught in schools). To illustrate my point practically, let me just ask you one question:
“How did science progress before modern schools and universities existed?”
The answer to this question lies at the core of this essay’s discussion. That answer is tinkering!
Intelligence Quotient Versus Trial and Error
Schools and universities optimise for top-down knowledge; they are environments where people with high-IQ (typically) flourish compared to others. In fact, the modern notion of IQ is pretty much a measure of how well someone performs mentally in comparison to others.
The IQ tests are said to be objective and neutral without any subject bias. I seriously doubt this claim. However, I am not going to go into that discussion in this essay. For there is no need for that discussion to outperform high-IQ individuals.
The key point to note is that the notion of IQ specifically measures one type of intelligence — a unitary individual’s mental pattern recognition ability in comparison to a cohort’s “average”. In other words, we are dealing with the deceptive and notorious normal distribution here; I am not a fan (and IQ is no longer even a quotient).
When you pit this notion of intelligence against rigorous trial and error, the results from trial and error are so far out ahead, that it does not even look like a fair comparison (more on that in a bit). Our current education system gives us the illusion that it pays well to sharpen the axe by spending time on learning theory before executing.
Trial and error (upgraded wheels v1) — Illustrative art created by the author
But real-life data and historical data suggest otherwise. It is not that high-IQ individuals have no access to trial and error. But it is the case that they are so incentivised by their good performance in top-down systems that they are more resistant to employing trial and error than the typical person.
So, where is the evidence for this outrageous real-life performance based on trial and error?
Where Does Trial and Error Flourish?
Most of the iconic and influential discoveries in science are made through trial and error. It is “after the fact” that scientists and theoreticians go back to “connect the dots” and make a story out of it. This gives modern theoreticians the illusion that science is top-down.
Historically, science has been driven by bottom-up knowledge. And “Trial and error” lies at the heart of bottom-up knowledge. You might think that this discussion would have been valid before the technological internet age, but not after.
All the big tech companies of our age hire people with very high-IQ after all. So, what gives? Well, look around you. Have you ever heard of the phrase “A/B testing” form big tech jargon before? Each and every feature of each and every successful app that you use was tested rigorously through trial and error.
If enough of us don’t pay attention to it, the feature goes away or is moved to where our attention is. This is why big tech companies value our data so much. It helps them run a monumental battery of trial and error tests which would never be possible via top-down theoretical approaches.
The modern venture of machine learning and artificial “intelligence” is proof that bottom-up knowledge is seriously undervalued and underrated. Some of these systems are now creating art that is beginning to rival human creations. And all of this is done without an inkling of theoretical understanding (on the part of the models). This brings us to the next part of our discussion.
How to Out Perform High IQ Individuals — The Unintelligent Vs. the Unintelligible
We often confuse the notion of the “unintelligent” with the “unintelligible”. Unintelligent means the lack of intelligence, whereas unintelligible means something that we cannot (or may never) understand.
We treat unintelligible modes of acquiring knowledge such as trial and error as unintelligent.
This is why today’s artificial intelligence systems are challenging many of our conventional beliefs and skills. If such unintelligible systems can dethrone today’s intelligent workers, we really have to ask ourselves the question:
“What IS intelligence, really?”
Why Does Trial and Error Outperform Top-down Systems?
The biggest advantage trial and error offers resides in the fact that the “errors” after each trial are optimised to be as small as possible. So, in this realm, there is almost no failure; only minor errors. And each time we encounter an error, we “learn” and “course correct” — the ideal goal of bottom-up knowledge.
Trial and error (upgraded wheels v2) — Illustrative art created by the author
This leads to a system where there are frequent (but small) errors and very infrequent (but massive) successes.
“It isn’t 10,000 hours that creates outliers, it’s 10,000 iterations.”
– Naval Ravikant.
In scientific terms, theory-based top-down knowledge works like a linear function, whereas trial and error-based bottom-up knowledge works like an exponential convex function. The differences are huge; the tech savvy reader might like to model these as randomised models and see the results for themselves.
Examples of Bottom-up Systems and Top-down Systems
We have already covered one example each of bottom-up systems (experimental/practical knowledge) and top-down systems (theoretical/academic knowledge). Here are some other examples of each of these systems:
1. Cooking (bottom-up) Vs. Baking (top-down) — If something doesn’t work out in cooking, it is very seldom that the entire dish is ruined. But the small error helps the next dish taste better. This is not the case with baking.
2. Entrepreneurs (bottom-up) Vs. MBA-Types (top-down) — The former makes numerous small mistakes and learns from each trial and error. On the other hand, the latter makes decisions on theoretical knowledge and doesn’t necessarily learn from error (due to the lack of incentives to do so).
3. Tinkerer Vs. Perfectionist — The former is interested in tinkering and seeing frequent results. On the other hand, the latter is focussed on executing perfectly in the first attempt, but takes a lot of time in planning for it.
I could go on and on with the examples, but you get the picture at this point. Now that I have clearly made the case for trial and error as a valuable source of (bottom-up) knowledge, it is time to answer the next big question.
Should We Demonise Top-down Knowledge?
After having rained down arrows on top-down theoretical knowledge, let me pose the next set of logical questions: Do I suggest demonising top-down academic knowledge? Should we completely get rid of it?
The answer is: no. I am not against top-down academic knowledge. What I am against is the propaganda of dressing up bottom-up knowledge as top-down knowledge.
“A lot of things that we believe come from theoretical knowledge, effectively come from tinkering.”
– Nassim Taleb.
Basic theoretical knowledge is indispensable for all of us, but at the same time, we need not be high-IQ geniuses to take advantage of it.
Use Trial and Error To Out Perform High IQ Individuals
The concepts that I have presented in this essay might upset modern theoreticians. But theoreticians, whilst very useful to society and science, should know their place. It is unfair and dishonourable to steal credit for knowledge/progress that was achieved through trial and error.
To the practitioner, this essay might come across as a welcome flag of approval. Although it is the case, I would like to caution that the key to a successful bottom-up knowledge model lies in designing it for “small” errors after each trial.
Dangers of trial and error — Illustrative art created by the author
The moment that errors get out of hand is the moment that the system stops being useful. If a system has a huge weak spot, it is bound to fail at that spot. So, design your systems carefully and acquire optimal theoretical knowledge where needed.
So, all in all, the tools of trial and error are available to everyone to benefit from. So, anyone can use them to outperform high-IQ individuals. The last time I checked, even high-IQ individuals wished that they outperformed other high-IQ individuals.
We use cookies on our website to give you the most relevant experience by remembering your preferences and repeat visits. By clicking “Accept”, you consent to the use of ALL the cookies.
This website uses cookies to improve your experience while you navigate through the website. Out of these, the cookies that are categorized as necessary are stored on your browser as they are essential for the working of basic functionalities of the website. We also use third-party cookies that help us analyze and understand how you use this website. These cookies will be stored in your browser only with your consent. You also have the option to opt-out of these cookies. But opting out of some of these cookies may affect your browsing experience.
Necessary cookies are absolutely essential for the website to function properly. These cookies ensure basic functionalities and security features of the website, anonymously.
Cookie
Duration
Description
cookielawinfo-checkbox-advertisement
1 year
Set by the GDPR Cookie Consent plugin, this cookie is used to record the user consent for the cookies in the "Advertisement" category .
cookielawinfo-checkbox-analytics
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Analytics".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-functional
11 months
The cookie is set by GDPR cookie consent to record the user consent for the cookies in the category "Functional".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-necessary
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookies is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Necessary".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-others
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Other.
cookielawinfo-checkbox-performance
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Performance".
CookieLawInfoConsent
1 year
Records the default button state of the corresponding category & the status of CCPA. It works only in coordination with the primary cookie.
viewed_cookie_policy
11 months
The cookie is set by the GDPR Cookie Consent plugin and is used to store whether or not user has consented to the use of cookies. It does not store any personal data.
Functional cookies help to perform certain functionalities like sharing the content of the website on social media platforms, collect feedbacks, and other third-party features.
Performance cookies are used to understand and analyze the key performance indexes of the website which helps in delivering a better user experience for the visitors.
Cookie
Duration
Description
_gat
1 minute
This cookie is installed by Google Universal Analytics to restrain request rate and thus limit the collection of data on high traffic sites.
Analytical cookies are used to understand how visitors interact with the website. These cookies help provide information on metrics the number of visitors, bounce rate, traffic source, etc.
Cookie
Duration
Description
__gads
1 year 24 days
The __gads cookie, set by Google, is stored under DoubleClick domain and tracks the number of times users see an advert, measures the success of the campaign and calculates its revenue. This cookie can only be read from the domain they are set on and will not track any data while browsing through other sites.
_ga
2 years
The _ga cookie, installed by Google Analytics, calculates visitor, session and campaign data and also keeps track of site usage for the site's analytics report. The cookie stores information anonymously and assigns a randomly generated number to recognize unique visitors.
_ga_R5WSNS3HKS
2 years
This cookie is installed by Google Analytics.
_gat_gtag_UA_131795354_1
1 minute
Set by Google to distinguish users.
_gid
1 day
Installed by Google Analytics, _gid cookie stores information on how visitors use a website, while also creating an analytics report of the website's performance. Some of the data that are collected include the number of visitors, their source, and the pages they visit anonymously.
CONSENT
2 years
YouTube sets this cookie via embedded youtube-videos and registers anonymous statistical data.
Advertisement cookies are used to provide visitors with relevant ads and marketing campaigns. These cookies track visitors across websites and collect information to provide customized ads.
Cookie
Duration
Description
IDE
1 year 24 days
Google DoubleClick IDE cookies are used to store information about how the user uses the website to present them with relevant ads and according to the user profile.
test_cookie
15 minutes
The test_cookie is set by doubleclick.net and is used to determine if the user's browser supports cookies.
VISITOR_INFO1_LIVE
5 months 27 days
A cookie set by YouTube to measure bandwidth that determines whether the user gets the new or old player interface.
YSC
session
YSC cookie is set by Youtube and is used to track the views of embedded videos on Youtube pages.
yt-remote-connected-devices
never
YouTube sets this cookie to store the video preferences of the user using embedded YouTube video.
yt-remote-device-id
never
YouTube sets this cookie to store the video preferences of the user using embedded YouTube video.
Comments